Recent Posts

Post 1: Observations

User:  | Open Learning Faculty Member: 


I have chosen to study two different areas within the Pacific Spirit Regional Park, Vancouver, B.C. This park occupies 874 ha, is predominantly forested and public access to the park is restricted to pedestrians and cyclists. The locations of the two different sites are presented in Figure 1. Site 1 is about 150m from the perimeter of the park between St George’s Trail and Sasamat trail. I have chosen to study is an area 10m by 10m located amongst the forest of Pacific Spirit Regional Park. The trees (mainly conifers) here are very high and mature and grow close together. There is a lot of moss and ferns, with bird song audible and is a very peaceful location. Figure 2 presents a photo of the area taken on April 17, at 6:40 pm. The temperature at the time was 10°C, the weather was lightly cloudy with a gentle breeze.

Figure 1. Site locations within Pacific Spirit Regional Park, Vancouver, B.B
Figure 2. Site 1 located in the interior of Pacific Spirit Regional Park

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Site 2 is located where Musqueam Creek (flowing south) leaves the park through a culvert under SW Marine Drive (a four lane road) following the creek north 25m, 2m either side of the river banks. The banks of the creek slope gently away from a sandy bed, with a variety of forest vegetation, including shrubs, ferns, and a variety of different sized trees, both conifers and deciduous trees. As the area is located next to a busy road there is traffic noise.  Photographs of the site were taken on April 13, 2018 at 9:00 am are presented in Figures 3 (looking north) and 4 (looking south). The temperature at the time was 9°C and lightly raining, with no wind.

Figure 3. Site 2 Musqueam Creek site looking north.

Figure 4. Site 2 Musqueam Creek site looking south

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Site 1 contrasts site 2 near Musqueam Creek as it is removed from the road, the trees are more closely spaced so there is less light through to the forest floor and the vegetation is more uniform. The Musqueam Creek site faces south and there are not so many large trees, and the large trees that are present are deciduous, so there is more light reaching the lower growing vegetation, and the vegetation in the area is more varied. Site 2 is also likely to have a higher moisture level in the soil as it is located on the banks of Musqueam Creek.  Some questions that could be asked are how does the vegetation differ between the two sites? For the ferns that are found in both locations, is there is difference in their size or density? Is there is a difference in the bird life between the two locations?

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Untitled

User:  | Open Learning Faculty Member: 


I chose to use a graph to display the data that I have collected. I used a line graph with two lines, one for grazing the 10 grazing replicates and one for the 10 non-grazing replicates, to show the average amounts of damage in percentage to each of the replicate areas.I chose to display it as an percentage of the overall area because I think it is easier for readers to understand and interpret. I think that the line graph I selected clearly displays the difference in damage between the two treatments and the overall trend. I did not have a difficult time organizing my data as I collected it in an excel spreadsheet so that it was quick and easy to enter equations and form graphs and tables from it. The data stayed with my predicted trend more or less, but I would be interested to explore the affect that the number of geese present grazing has on the percent of area damaged. When I first visited the site sometime ago there were only 2-3 geese grazing, but on the day I collected the 20 replicates there were 5. I’d assume that the increase in geese presence would also increase the % of area damaged.

Blog 6 Data Collection

User:  | Open Learning Faculty Member: 


Overcast, humidity 79%, Wind from the SW 18 km/hr, 9 degrees Celsius

April 16, 2018

I will continue counting cone totals in the red squirrel midden and cones on conifer trees within a 20 meter radius of the squirrel’s central midden.  My hpothesis is that a red squirrel in an urban habitat has the same amount of  conifer cones within a 20 meter radius and the same amount of cones  stashed in his winter midden as a red squirrel has in the forest.

My field study is a continuation of Donald Paul Streubels phd thesis “Food storing and related behavior of red squirrels in Interior Alaska”  May, 1968.

Because Streubel’s  study was done in a forest environment I will be modifying my study to the conifer trees within an urban environment, as such, I will be focusing on the 16 mature conifer cone producing trees within a 40 diameter of the wood pile which is the red squirrel’s midden.  I will count the cones dropped from each tree to an extension of 2 meters past the longest branch and all the cones under the tree as well as all of the cones still on the trees.  I will continue counting the cones in the woodpile as I move the wood to my wood shed.  I am finding eaten cones, whole opened cones, closed cones, along with cones buried under the wood pallets to a depth of 10 cm. and all will be included in the count.

My prediction is that a red squirrel choses a central midden in an urban environment that equals his wild counterpart’s midden in the forest in terms of a 20 meter accessiblilty to mature conifer cone producing trees and conifer cones as well as storing the same amount of conifer cones required for winter survival in his central midden.

Response Variable:  Total number of cones in the red squirrel’s central midden.  Categorical

Predictor Variable:  Number of cones underneath the conifer trees and on the conifer trees within a 20 meter radius of the red squirrel midden.  Continuous

Experimental Design:  Logistic Regression

Midden:  a food stash containing conifer cones along with piles of scales from cones that have already been eaten by the squirrel.

 

Blog Post 9: Field Research Reflections

User:  | Open Learning Faculty Member: 


I found the process of carrying out this research project to be quite interesting and educational.  My single biggest challenge was coming up with a topic as there are so many interesting things to study!  My next biggest challenge was taking the theory of an experimental design and actually implementing it in the field.  I ended up changing my overall design a couple of times.  For those not following my research project, I had looked at the differences in the frequency of occurrence and cover of species of moss on different slope positions of rock outcrops.

I had originally thought that I would use separate rock outcrops as the replicates, but I only found 4 outcrops in the area that had similar enough attributes to be compared as replicates, which did not meet the rule of 10.  I therefore ended up collecting 10 samples in each slope position from among the different rock outcrops to serve as the replicates.

I had also originally intended to use a transect along which to locate evenly spaced plots within each rock outcrop, but I found that this restricted my sample locations too much given the variation in the orientation of the slope positions and it did not enable me to collect enough samples in the narrower slope positions (particularly the crests).  I therefore switched to a strategic randomized selection in which I sampled an equal number of randomly selected plot locations within each slope position, in order to get a sufficient sample size from each slope position.

This process certainly gave me an appreciation of the complexity of implementing ecological experiments, particularly in a field setting, where it is sometimes difficult (e.g. costly or time-consuming), if not impossible (e.g. conditions have changed or species have moved on), to repeat any data collection.  It is also very difficult to control for external environmental variables, which can interact with one another and vary across even small distances.

 

 

Blog Post 6: Data Collection

User:  | Open Learning Faculty Member: 


I have made several changes to my data collection strategy since last time. While I am still using a haphazard sampling method with the help of Google maps, and point counts for estimating Black-billed magpie abundance in relation to human presence in Edmonton’s Hermitage park, I have moved to a dependent double-observer approach. Also, I have reduced the time frame spent at each point count to 20 minutes. There was an additional one-minute settling time given at each point, and five minutes were given to walk from one point to another. Additionally, I have chosen to assess human presence solely by recording the number of pedestrians observed during given time frame. Doing so resulted, I believe, in less bias than recording various predefined “human traces”.  Moreover, this time, I chose to record separately: 1. No. of birds seen within the first 5 minutes, 2. No. of birds seen flying over. Doing so will give the reader an idea of whether the presence of observers might have attracted more birds after the first five minutes. Birds seen flying over are generally noted separately in current similar literature, because they cannot be recorded in standard density calculations (Gregory et al. 2004).

To increase the accuracy of my results, I have increased the number of point counts from five to nine which I sampled on two different days (March 24th and March 28th 2018).

As I will explain further in my final report, my literature review as well as previous comments on my past assignments has led me to make those changes. My intention was to minimize bias, standardize my sampling as much as possible, and increase the use of randomization/replication in my sampling strategy. Using a dependent double-observer approach made it much easier to implement my sampling design. Not only did it reduce observer bias, less coordination was needed than it was using different observers for each point count. The only problem encountered was that most of the time, the walking time took a bit less than five minutes, so the real “settling time” was more than a full minute at most points. It was hard to keep track of the exact time spent at each location for that reason.

Gregory, R. D., D. W. Gibbons, and P. F. Donalds. 2004. Bird census and survey techniques. Pages 17–55 in R. E. Green, W. J. Sutherland, and I. Newton. Bird ecology and conservation: a handbook of techniques. Oxford University Press, New York, New York, USA.

Blog 8: Summary Table

User:  | Open Learning Faculty Member: 


To summarize my findings for the richness of healthy living moss found on the trunks of trees I had to use a number scale to help average out my finds to show any difference in the moss richness found on the three tree top coverage categories. Once it was decided that a tree with hardly no moss present would be given a value of zero, a tree with some healthy moss present was 0.5, and a tree with high moss richness was 1.0 it was easy to summarize the data collected.

As shown in table 1 the partial tree top exposer category showed the highest average of moss richness, this went against the prediction that moss richness would be highest at the shelter tree top group. Though it was shown that the exposed tree top group did have the lowest moss richness where it was about half the amount of the other two tree categories which supports the prediction that the exposed tree top group would have the lowest moss richness. As the data was being collected over the two weeks it was observed that the partial tree top exposer group had increased the moss richness on the tree trunk the most noticeably. Where it is thought the partial tree top exposer will have the fastest rate of increased moss richness on the tree trunks when entering the spring season in British Columbia when compared to the other two tree groups. However, more data collected over a longer time period during the late winter and spring season is needed to test this prediction.

Tree Top Exposer Total Average Moss Richness
Exposed 0.125
Partial 0.288
Sheltered 0.250
Table 1: Total average of  healthy living moss found on the tree trunks of the three tree top exposer categories. Each tree top category contained ten replicates where date was collected four times at each site over the spanned of two weeks. A number value of 0.0, 0.5, and 1.0 was used to stand for no moss richness, some moss richness, and high moss richness found on each of the tree trunks respectively.

Blog Post 7: Theoretical Perspectives

User:  | Open Learning Faculty Member: 


The main ecological process in which my hypothesis touches upon is Herbivory. Herbivory is the consumption of plant material by animals, and herbivores are animals that are adapted to eat plants. Herbivory is a type of predation-prey interaction, which in this the case the Canada Geese is the predator and the Kentucky Bluegrass is the prey. Many plants, specifically weeds, in the park area have evolved defenses like chemicals and thorns to protect themselves from the predators, so the Canada Geese must be careful to pick the less defensive plants, such as the Kentucky Bluegrass. Another process which the hypothesis relates to is the reproductive (and evolutionary) fitness of the Kentucky Bluegrass species in the park area. The hypothesis speaks to the level of damage inflicted by Canada Geese on Kentucky Bluegrass in the grazing area relative to the level of damage in a non-grazing area. This shows how the Canada Geese grazing habits are affecting the fitness of the Kentucky Bluegrass. I have predicted that the study will show that overall the grazing habits of Canada Geese will cause more damage to the grass species than if there was no grazing at all. This damage will decrease the amount of grass in the area and therefore reduce the reproductive fitness of the Kentucky Bluegrass species. Three keywords that I would use to describe my research project would be:

  1.  Canada Goose Grazing Habits
  2. Herbivory Interaction of Canada Goose and Kentucky Bluegrass
  3. Managing Negative Impacts of Canada Geese

Blog Post 6: Data Collection

User:  | Open Learning Faculty Member: 


I have made several revisions to my sampling in the field. While the overall strategy will stay the same, haphazard with subjective location selection, I will be decreasing the size of my quadrats from 100cm x 100cm to 30cm x 30cm.This reduction will allow for quicker observation of the damaged area and give an overall clearer picture. In addition to the reduction of my quadrats I will now be looking at the damage to the Kentucky Bluegrass species instead of the Ryegrass species. With the turn from winter to spring, Kentucky Bluegrass has shown itself to be the dominant species in the grassy area. Also, I will now be taking 10 replicate samples, 5 grazing and 5 non-grazing, in order to satisfy the rule of ten. In satisfying the rule of ten I hope to achieve a more accurate representation of the ecological processes that are occurring. There were no major difficulties in implementing the original sampling design, but I believe that these changes will allow for a more streamline process. There is one ancillary pattern that may have an effect on the original hypothesis. This pattern is the increasing presence of the number of geese in the grazing areas with increasingly warm weather. It appears that on warm and sunny days the number of geese in the grazing area of the park will increase by anywhere from 2 to 8 geese. While this does not change the fact that the geese are damaging the Kentucky Bluegrass in the area, it is worth observing the damage done on days where geese numbers are higher versus the damage done on days where geese numbers are at 2 or 3.

 

 Figure 1: Warm Weather Brings an Increased Number of Geese to Feed at McMaster University.

Blog Post 7: Theoretical Perspectives

User:  | Open Learning Faculty Member: 


When I had observed the wildlife in the pond for the first time I had noticed there were no ducks in the shallow areas. I had seen a muskrat swimming in the shallow water. Therefore, the question I had was in regards to predator-prey interactions. I had thought that the ducks purposely chose to avoid the shallow water to avoid the possibility of being attacked by a predator. However, after sampling, I found the ducks sometimes chose to stay in shallow water and perhaps the predator-prey interactions were not the driving force behind the choice of location within a pond. Therefore I am investigating one of the many potential factors influencing the patch choice of the mallard ducks and redhead ducks. Therefore, I feel my topic covers the keywords below:

Keywords: Patch Choice; Foraging behaviour; Animal Nestings; Animal behaviour

Blog Post 6: Data Collection

User:  | Open Learning Faculty Member: 


I had been collecting data March 26 to March 28 on the number of ducks in the three locations (Land, Shallow water, Deep water) and found there to be somewhat more ducks in the deep water. I had collected a total of 5 samples. Sample 2 and 3 were on the same day at different times. Sample 4 and 5 were also on the same day at different times. During these sampling periods, I noticed something important: the ducks appear to change preference throughout the day. This data is simplified and shown in Table 1 below. Therefore,  I had collected 4 more days of data at three points in time during the week of April 2nd. This data has yet to be analyzed – but this had changed my initial hypothesis. I had originally hypothesized that the ducks prefer to be in the deep water at all times but I hypothesize that they prefer the shallow water during certain light levels.

Table 1. The average number of ducks seen in each location throughout the day for each sampling trial. Each trial shows the mean number of ducks in the location during the sampling hour. The Average number is shown in the last row.

Trial Land Shallow Deep
1 0.0 10.3 8.6
2 0.2 2.9 9.4
3 0.0 6.0 6.5
4 0.4 3.9 10.3
5 0.0 6.6 6.8
0.1 6.0 8.3